Identifying patterns of motor performance, executive functioning, and verbal ability in preschool children: A latent profile analysis
  • Date: 10 Sep, 2019
  • Throughout development, children become increasingly more able to control their motor actions (Hamilton, Southgate, & Hill,

    2016). This developing motor control represents planning, organizing, monitoring, and controlling complex motor coordination

    which seem to have an intuitive connection with executive functioning (EF). Although the exact definition is widely discussed, most

    researchers would agree on the notion that EF refers to a set of higher-order cognitive processes, such as inhibition, working memory,

    and cognitive flexibility, which are instrumental in supporting action control and thought (e.g., Carlson, Faja, & Beck, 2016). The

    conceptual overlap has been highlighted previously: by definition, purposive movement involves action control, and action control is

    an essential part of EF (Koziol, Budding, & Chidekel, 2012). Another key developmental skill to consider when exploring the relationship

    between motor performance and EF is verbal ability. Verbalizing thoughts supports action control, i.e., expression of

    actions, reflection of performed actions, and planning of future actions (Kray, Eenshuistra, Kerstner, Weidema, & Hommel, 2006).

    The idea that there is a relationship between motor performance and higher-order cognitive functions, such as EF and language,

    stems also in part from theoretical perspectives. For example, in the embodied cognition perspective, cognition – and EF and language

    as subdomains of cognition – are considered to occur in the context of the individual's bodily interaction with the physical and social

    environment (Barsalou, 1999; Gibbs, 2005; Smith & Gasser, 2005). Being able to act upon their environment allows children to gain

    knowledge about their surroundings, which leads to changes in various perception-action systems (Von Hofsten, 2009). These

    changes bring about advances in cognition that in turn will affect how children examine and manipulate their environment (Campos

    et al., 2000; Von Hofsten, 2007). This is not to say that the physical body is the only system involved in cognition or that one can

    assume a global association between motor performance and cognition, but that specific motor actions could play a role in this process

    (Oudgenoeg-Paz, Volman, & Leseman, 2016).

    In addition, several authors have argued that thought, reasoning, and other forms of complex cognitive processes, such as EF,

    depend on interiorization of actions (Ardila, 2012). Verbal ability may be viewed as an essential means in the interiorization of

    actions. For instance, a central point in Vygotsky’s theory (1962) is that verbal ability represents a major instrument of internal

    representation of the world and thinking. Similarly, Clark (2008) describes language as cognitive scaffolding, extending the embodied

    mind and making it possible to generalize across situations and experiences. Verbal ability thus helps children to regulate their own

    actions and thoughts.

    The conceptual and theoretical link between the aforementioned developmental domains is supported by neuroimaging research

    (Diamond, 2000; Pangelinan et al., 2011; Pulvermüller, 2005). Brain areas associated with EF, such as the dorsolateral prefrontal

    cortex, and brain areas necessary for the planning and execution of movements, such as the cerebellum and basal ganglia, are coactivated

    during the execution of specific motor and EF tasks (Diamond, 2000). For example, a study examining brain activity in 8- to

    12-year old children during a motor task showed activation of a broad network of regions, including the dorsolateral prefrontal

    cortex, inferior parietal lobule, and the cerebellum (Zwicker, Missiuna, Harris, & Boyd, 2011). Furthermore, areas of the brain

    implicated in language functions (e.g., Broca’s area) are also activated during EF tasks (Gerton et al., 2004) and motor tasks (i.e.,

    action planning, action observation, action understanding, and imitation; Nishitani, Schürmann, Amunts, & Hari, 2005). In addition,

    the activation of motor areas has been observed during language tasks (e.g., Casado et al., 2018; Pulvermüller, 2005; Willems &

    Hagoort, 2007).

    Yet behavioural studies looking at direct connections between developmental domains have not yielded clear results. Studies

    examining the relationship between motor performance and EF have revealed only modest associations between both domains,

    including studies that do not find these associations (Hamilton et al., 2016; Van der Fels et al., 2015). In addition, although studies

    reported that children with motor coordination difficulties, including children with a diagnosis of Developmental Coordination

    Disorder (DCD), have clear EF difficulties (Leonard & Hill, 2015; Wilson, Ruddock, Smits-Engelsman, Polatajko, & Blank, 2013),

    research has also shown that the commonly assumed link between motor coordination difficulties and executive dysfunction is not

    always present (Molitor, Michel, & Schneider, 2015).

    Similarly, while in recent years increasing empirical evidence is reported for a link between motor performance and language in

    typically developing children during the first 3 years of life (e.g., He, Walle, & Campos, 2015; Libertus & Violi, 2016; Walle & Campos,

    2014), the relationship seems to weaken or disappear as a function of age (Libertus & Hauf, 2017; Oudgenoeg-Paz et al., 2016). In

    addition, developmental disorders such as Specific Language Impairment (SLI) have been related to motor coordination difficulties

    and DCD to language impairments (see Hill, 2001 and Leonard & Hill, 2014 for reviews). Although the general finding in children

    with developmental disorders is one of relatively high rates of co-occurrence between motor coordination difficulties and language

    impairments, not all children with motor coordination difficulties have language impairments and vice versa.

    With regard to the relationship between verbal ability and EF, a number of studies have shown that typically developing children’s

    verbal ability is related with EF performance, and that children with SLI score poorly on EF tasks (e.g., Fuhs & Day, 2011;

    Gooch, Thompson, Nash, Snowling, & Hulme, 2016; Kaushanskaya, Park, Gangopadhyay, Davidson, & Weismer, 2017; see Müller,

    Jacques, Brocki & Zelazo, 2009 for a review). However, with regard to the EF of children with SLI, conflicting results are also present

    with some researchers finding evidence of dysfunction in children with SLI and others reporting equivalent performance between

    S. Houwen et al. Research in Developmental Disabilities 84 (2019) 3–15

    4

    children with SLI and typically developing children (Kapa & Plante, 2015).

    Overall, studies are inconclusive about the exact extent of the relationships between developmental domains, which might be due

    to developmental differences within children, individual differences across children, and to different measures used to assess children’s

    motor performance, EF, and verbal ability (Leonard & Hill, 2015; Libertus & Hauf, 2017). Furthermore, despite empirical

    associations between motor performance, EF, and verbal ability in different bivariate combinations, no study has yet explored how

    these three domains interact. This research is the first to examine these three areas of child development concurrently in early

    childhood in order to shed light on the constellations of motor performance, EF, and verbal ability in young children.

    Mainly variable-oriented and correlational methods have been used when examining the relationship between the aforementioned

    developmental domains, which have been based on the assumption of linearity of relationships. Such an approach potentially

    oversimplifies the complex interplay between developmental domains in young children. The possibility that subgroups of individuals

    may show profiles with different interrelations between motor performance, EF, and/or verbal ability has rarely been taken

    into account. A person-centered approach, such as latent profile analysis (LPA), can describe the patterning of multiple variables

    within individuals to capture essential features of functioning that may be lost when simple linear associations are analysed (Bergman

    & Magnusson, 1997; Collins & Lanza, 2010). Using a person-centered approach for examining the relationship between motor

    performance, EF, and verbal ability may be vital at all stages of child development, but even more so during the preschool-age period

    because this developmental period is characterized by both a rapid growth as well as considerable intra- and interindividual

    variability in motor performance, EF, and verbal ability (Howard, Okely, & Ellis, 2015; Piek, Hands, & Licari, 2012).

    An important issue to consider when examining EF in preschool children is the structure of EF. For primary school children,

    adolescents, and adults there is conclusive evidence concerning three distinguishable, yet interrelated, constructs of EF; that is:

    working memory, inhibition, and cognitive flexibility (Huizinga, Dolan, & van der Molen, 2006; Miyake, Emerson, & Friedman,

    2000). Yet, in preschool aged children the evidence regarding the structure of EF is less conclusive. Factor analysis studies with 3-year

    olds consistently show a unitary EF factor model (Wiebe, Espy, & Charak, 2008; Willoughby, Wirth, & Blair, 2012). Both one- and

    two-factor models have been found within samples of 4- and 5-year old preschoolers, with the majority of two-factor models revealing

    an inhibition and working memory component (e.g., Lee, Bull, & Ho, 2013; Monette, Bigras, & Lafrenière, 2015). It is

    important to note however, that in many of these two-factor models working memory and inhibition were significantly correlated

    (with correlations>.80; Monette et al., 2015). Additionally, it is argued that cognitive flexibility is only emerging from the primary

    school age and thus not yet distinguishable in preschool aged children (e.g., Garon, Bryson, & Smith, 2008). In sum, empirical

    evidence seems to support an initial unitary structure of EF, and as a function of age a differentiation of components occurs.

    Related to the structure of EF, is the operationalization and measurement of EF, specifically performance-based measures versus

    parent ratings of EF (Leonard & Hill, 2015; Toplak, West, & Stanovich, 2013). There is mounting evidence that performance-based

    measures and parent ratings of EF do not assess the same construct (Toplak et al., 2013). While performance-based EF measures

    typically assess specific, individual executive functions under highly structured and standardized conditions, rating scales of EF were

    developed to tap into complex, real-world manifestations of executive functions (Toplak et al., 2013). It has been suggested that the

    type of EF measurement may affect the relationship found between motor performance and EF (Houwen, van der Veer, Visser, &

    Cantell, 2017; Ten Eycke & Dewey, 2016). Addressing this issue is particularly relevant in early childhood, where assessment of EF by

    performance-based measures is a challenge, as validated tests are relatively few, norms are uncertain, and variables as limited

    attention span, motivation, and confidence in the testing situation might influence the results (Nilsen, Huyder, McAuley, &

    Liebermann, 2016).

    Therefore, the first aim of the current study was to identify distinct constellations of motor skills, EF, and verbal ability in

    preschool aged children. For EF both multiple performance-based measures and a parent rating measure were used. Based on the

    reviewed evidence and our young sample, we expected a one-factor model for EF including inhibition and working memory tasks. As

    performance-based EF measures and parent ratings of EF have been found to provide complementary but distinctive information, we

    assumed a model with a unitary EF variable based on performance-based tasks and a unitary EF variable involving the parent rating.

    We have chosen to focus on receptive vocabulary as a measure of verbal ability, as receptive vocabulary develops rapidly in early

    childhood and builds the foundation for language acquisition and literacy (Powell & Diamond, 2012). Furthermore, it has been

    suggested that receptive vocabulary tests provide the purest measure of language ability (Milligan, Astington, & Dack, 2007). Given

    the exploratory and innovative nature of our study, it was impossible to have a comprehensive view on the number of possible

    profiles. We did, however expect to delineate profiles that not only differ in quantity, but exhibit qualitatively distinct patterns of

    motor, EF, and verbal ability skills. The second aim was to explore how individual and contextual variables were related to profile

    membership.

  • Documents: